The Dimensions of Colour: Brightness and Colourfulness

1.6 The Dimensions of Colour: Brightness and Colourfulness

Dimensions of Colours of Light

Figure 1.6.1. Digital painting of a stripe of red paint passing between shadow and light.

Consider a stripe of red paint passing between shadow and light (Fig. 1.6.1). By virtue of the unconscious processes that equip our vision with a high degree of object-colour constancy under varying intensities of the same illumination, we instantly, effortlessly and automatically see the stripe as being the same colour, that is, as having the same red object colour, over its whole length. This red colour can be specified more precisely in terms of an object colour notation such as Munsell hue, value and chroma, and we could confirm that the stripe matches the same Munsell chip placed beside it in the shadow and in the light. Nevertheless, the stripe appears brighter and more colourful in the light than in the shadow. This difference in brightness and colourfulness relates to the appearance of the light reaching our eyes from different areas of the object.
The CIE definitions of brightness and colourfulness are:
Brightness: "attribute of a visual perception according to which an area appears to emit, or reflect, more or less light. NOTE The use of this term is not restricted to primary light sources" (CIE, 2011, 17-111).
Colourfulness: "attribute of a visual perception according to which the perceived colour of an area appears to be more or less chromatic" (CIE, 2011, 17-233).
In our example of the red paint stripe the colour attributes of brightness and colourfulness pertain to the perceived colour of the light reaching our eyes from different parts of the stripe, rather than to the object colour seen as belonging to the stripe itself. So while perceived colours of objects can be described in terms of just three dimensions such as hue, lightness and chroma, perceived colours of lights are described in terms of different sets of three dimensions, such as hue, brightness and colourfulness, or alternatively as discussed in the next section, hue, brightness and saturation. Describing the colour appearance of an illuminated object involves both types of colour, and so requires more than three dimensions.

Figure 1.6.2. A White's illusion: the grey bars all have the same physical luminance, but are perceived to differ in brightness and lightness. B. Fig. 1.6.2A viewed through a screen with two apertures. C. Light-shedding illusion by Akiyoshi Kitaoka from his website Akiyoshi's Illusion Pages: the central area is identical in luminance to the white surrounding the figure, but appears brighter and thus luminous. D. Fig. 1.6.2C viewed through a screen with two apertures.

Brightness is our perception of luminance (visible light energy), whether of a primary light source or of a light-reflecting or light-transmitting object. Perception of absolute luminance is strongly influenced by the state of brightness adaptation of the observer: for example, your laptop screen is comfortably bright when viewed indoors but very dim outdoors, even though it has exactly the same physical luminance. Perception of the relative luminance of a given area in the visual field is influenced by the luminance of the immediate surround (contrast phenomena) and of finely interspersed areas (assimilation). Contrast phenomena (e.g. simultaneous contrast and Mach bands) tend to push the brightness and other perceived colour attributes of an area away from those of the immediate surround, while assimilation tends to move the perceived colour attributes of an area towards those of finely interspersed areas. In some carefully contrived examples like White's illusion (Fig. 1.6.2A) and light-shedding illusions (Fig. 1.6.2C) effects of this kind can result in particularly large misperceptions of relative luminance. Misperceptions of this kind are not eliminated by "squinting" (which actually increases effects involving assimilation like White's illusion), and in general observers can only perceive relative luminance veridically (that is, as being equal in the areas concerned) by using an opaque screen with two small apertures (Fig. 1.6.2.B,D).

Figure 1.6.3. Above: Sunrise, Blayney. David Briggs, 2014, oil on canvas, 12" by 24". Below: detail of Sunrise, Blayney viewed through a screen with two apertures, showing approximate equality of brightness of the areas depicting the sun and the brightest parts of the clouds.

In the landscape painting Sunrise, Blayney an attempt was made to use the image colour relationships built into light-shedding illusions to create an impression of luminosity in the area depicting the sun, even though this area is painted with the same white paint as the brightest of the clouds on the left.
A variety of sophisticated quantitative measures of brightness and colourfulness have been devised in modern colour appearance models, including the Nayatani et al., Hunt and CIECAM02 models (see Fairchild, 2013 for detailed accounts). Painters however measure variations in brightness and colourfulness in a real or imagined scene in terms of the lightness and chroma of the paints they will use to represent that scene. Indeed, looking a scene like that depicted in Fig. 1.6.1, many painters would simply say that the lightness and the chroma of the stripe increase from left to right, automatically thinking in terms of the colours of the paints they would use. However the painter's representation of brightness and colourfulness as lightness and chroma, like the photographer's, involves choosing consciously or otherwise from a range of possible scaling strategies, for example "crowding" the lights or the darks (Pope, 1922).

Seeing Brightness

Figure 1.6.4. Checker shadow illusion created by Edward H. Adelson. (click to enlarge) Source: http://web.mit.edu/persci/people/adelson/checkershadow_illusion.html. Adelson's caption on the preceding web link ("The squares marked A and B are the same shade of gray") slyly misdirects us. The squares marked A and B "exist" only in the scene depicted in the image, where they have different object colours; it is the rhomboidal image areas labelled A and B that have the same colour and luminance, difficult as it may be to attend to this until the representational spell of the image is broken (right).

Perceiving brightness is not as straightforward a task as it might first appear. By virtue of the pre-conscious processes responsible for object-colour constancy, our perception of lightness is normally immediate, effortless and automatic. We clearly also have an immediate sense of variations of brightness within the visual field in a general way, but the task of making precise comparisons of brightness between different parts of the visual field proves to be surprisingly difficult. We discover this both when we are learning to paint and when we look at optical illusions such as Adelson's well-know checker shadow illusion (Fig. 1.6.4, left). The image areas A and B have the same luminance, but because we automatically see A as a dark surface and B as a light surface it is extraordinarily difficult to see and compare the colours of the image in these areas. Even if we consciously observe that B is a light coloured surface in a shadow and that A is a dark coloured surface in light, and therefore rationally infer that the two areas must at least be more similar in brightness than they are in lightness, it remains stubbornly difficult to see this similarity, let alone to see that they are identical in brightness. Our attention is so firmly held by these pre-consciously inferred differences in lightness that we literally cannot see the identical brightness of the areas until we break the spell of the image by connecting them up (Fig. 1.6.4, right).
It is commonplace in teaching painting that beginning students paint objects in something closer to their local colour than to the colour that they need to be to look to have this local colour in the painting. As non-painters, our conscious visual perceptions primarily track properties of objects like reflectance rather than properties of the visual field like luminance. There is nothing peculiar about this: in evolutionary terms the primary value of our visual system is in providing information about objects in the world rather than raw information about the visual field. Our visual system clearly collects information about relative luminance accurately enough to permit pre-conscious "inference" of reasonably stable object colour properties (however this is done!), but we are not accustomed to accessing this relative luminance information consciously. We do not need to do so until we start to paint.
Faced with this difficulty, painters have developed various strategies to help them to make more accurate judgements of relative brightness. One of the best known is "squinting", that is, viewing through the eyelashes of nearly closed eyes. This has the effect of flattening the 3D model of object colours and illumination that our brain has so cleverly disentangled for us into a blurred two-dimensional array of light (watch how things jump back in space the instant you open your eyes!). Alternative techniques that work for some observers include staring at a fixed point and making comparisons using peripheral vision, or letting the eyes wander casually around the visual field. ("unfocussing the eyes"). With a little persistence at least one of these methods allows most students to perceive (often quite suddenly) the equality of luminance of A and B in Fig. 1.6.4. Even if all of these strategies fail, the use of a screen with two apertures allows every student to perceive the relative luminance of the two areas veridically.

Figure 1.6.5. Form illusion by R. Beau Lotto (click to enlarge).. The two image shapes depicting tables have the same proportions measured through the middle . For an interactive version of this and many other illusions visit Lotto's website at http://www.labofmisfits.com/articles/illusionsoflight.asp

An analogous situation exists with shape constancy. In Fig. 1.6.5 we can see without difficulty that both of the rectangular tables depicted present a trapezoidal shape in the visual field, but it is extraordinarily difficult to attend to these areas as flat shapes in the picture plane enough to see that they have exactly the same proportions measured through their centres. For misperceptions like this induced by shape constancy it is the artists's strategy of measuring with a pencil or paintbrush that comes to the rescue, allowing proportions in the picture plane to be determined veridically.

Alternative Definitions and Usages

The current CIE definition of brightness explicitly states that the term is not restricted to primary light sources. Previously the 1970 edition of the CIE International Lighting Vocabulary (as quoted by Wyszecki and Stiles, 1982) had defined brightness as "the attribute of a visual sensation according to which a given visual stimulus appears to be more or less intense, or according to which the area in which a visual stimulus is presented appears to emit more or less light". While the word "emit" here might be interpreted to imply a primary light source, this clearly was not the intended meaning because the same word is used in the accompanying definition of lightness as the extent to which an area "appears to emit more or less light in proportion to that emitted by a similarly illuminated area perceived as a 'white' stimulus".
Arend (1993) published more concise definitions of brightness and lightness characterized by the author as "similar to the CIE and OSA definitions, but slightly clarified and simplified". Brightness was defined as "apparent luminance, the apparent amount of light coming from a visual direction" and lightness as "apparent reflectance". The simplified definition again does not restrict the term "brightness" to primary light sources.
In contrast, Marr (1982) and Palmer (1999) have used "brightness" and "lightness"to refer to perceptions of primary light sources and of light-reflecting objects respectively in the context of certain stages of visual perception. I am grateful to Anthony Waichulis for drawing my attention to these non-standard usages.

Figure 1.6.6. Test stimulus similar to one illustrated by Purves and Lotto (2011), after Blakeslee and McCourt (2015) The four diamond-shaped areas are identical in physical luminance and are perceived as similarly light grey image colours (about 80 on the included scale), but due to simultaneous contrast those on the darker background are seen by many observers as slightly brighter, and lighter in image colour, than those on the lighter background. The two diamond shapes on the darker background in A and B appear a very similar or identical light grey image colour, and thus are similar or identical in brightness in its standard sense, but considered as object-colours depicted in the scenes they differ greatly in lightness: light grey in A and much brighter than a similarly illuminated white and thus self-luminous in B.

Purves and Lotto (2011, p.17) describe lightness as "the lighter or darker appearance of object surfaces" and brightness as "the brighter or dimmer appearance of objects that are sources of light (e.g. the sun, fire, lightbulbs etc.)". From that point on they use the terms either interchangeably or combined in the expression "lightness/brightness", and they discuss the "peculiar relationship between lightness/ brightness and luminance" in a series of examples, most of which are images depicting three-dimensional scenes like Fig. 1.6.6. Lightness and brightness in its standard sense are simply correlated only in two-dimensional patterns and in images of uniformly lit planes. In all other images there is an important distinction between the proximal property of image-colour lightness and the distal property of the perceived lightness of objects depicted in the scene (Fig. 1.6.1, 1.6.6). What Purves and Lotto mean by "lightness/ brightness" in images representing three-dimensional scenes is therefore not obvious, but in their examples it seems to track an overall impression of lightness or luminosity that is an unexamined conflation of image-colour and object-colour lightness, aligning more closely with one or the other according to how convincingly their image depicts a three-dimensional scene. Unsurprisingly this impression of "lightness/ brightness" correlates very poorly with physical luminance, and to account for its vagaries and those of similar conflations of proximal and distal properties ("the angles between these rods look different but they are actually the same" etc.), Purves and Lotto propose a new empirical theory of vision in which visual perceptions "correspond to the operational link between the stimulus and a successful behavioral response, discovered over the course of evolutionary and individual time". Their model assumes however that empirical input does not merely influence our overall visual impressions but entirely defines all that we can ever see.
As stated previously, the visual world is not a direct "window on reality" but is more like a 3D computer model consisting of mind-generated perceived attributes, including colour attributes of objects and illumination, projected onto the external world. It's certainly reasonable to suppose that what we ordinarily notice about this visual world is what has been useful to us in our personal and evolutionary past, namely the unconsciously inferred properties of objects. For this reason what we tend to notice in a convincing image of a three-dimensional scene is the inferred lightness of objects in that scene and not the lightness of the image itself. But there is an important difference between what we ordinarily notice in the visual world and what we can learn to see.
Finally, there is a usage common both colloquially and in the textile industry in which white, strong colours, and all intermediate tints are all described as "bright". These are the colours that form the upward-facing surface of most colour spaces including the Munsell system. Corresponding digital colours are said to have a "brightness" B of 100 in HSB colour space because they each represent the brightest possible digital colour for a given hue and saturation (Fig. 1.7.6). In NCS terminology such colours have little or no blackness (Section 1.8) and in Evans' (1974) terminology they have approximately zero greyness (Section 1.9).

Page added January 30, 2017.

<< 12 3 4 5 6 7 8 >>
Next: The Dimensions of Colour: Saturation